How motivation and engagement in the learning environment can influence self-regulated learners

Introduction

Electronic learning [or e-learning] has gained increased attention during the COVID-19 lockdown. It has facilitated student learning and well-being by providing Internet access to teaching and learning programs [Radha et al., 2020] and psychological assistance [Zhao et al., 2020] in universities and schools. However, in many developing countries, e-learning is currently underused and still in its infant phase. A lack of use is resulting in resource wastage and the failure of e-learning systems generally [Almaiah et al., 2020]. Therefore, improving learners’ behavioral intention to reuse e-learning systems is a major challenge for many universities [Alqahtani and Rajkhan, 2020; Radha et al., 2020].

During the past decades, scholars have identified several factors affecting learners’ adoption of the e-learning systems: performance expectancy, effort expectancy, social influence, and facilitating conditions [Zhang et al., 2020]. Indeed, learning environment and learning expectations are associated with higher behavioral intention to reuse. However, e-learning might require a set of different strategies and skills to attain learning resources. Therefore, we focus on one specific learning strategy, namely, self-regulation. Self-regulated learning refers to a series of active and volitional behaviors that individual uses to accomplish a learning goal [Zimmerman, 1986; Panadero, 2017]. Several studies have indicated that self-regulation can help the learner to control, manage, and plan their e-learning [Panadero, 2017; van Alten et al., 2020a]. Nevertheless, the link between self-regulation and behavioral intention to reuse has rarely been investigated. Thus, it is crucial to explore the impacts of self-regulation on behavioral intention to reuse in the online environment.

The present study aimed to examine the effect of self-regulation on learners’ behavioral intention to reuse using the conservation of resource theory [Hobfoll, 1989]. We proposed that when learners have the self-generated ability to control, manage, and plan the learning process, they show a greater behavioral intention to reuse e-learning systems. Moreover, we posited that self-regulation may be positively related to behavioral intention to reuse via study engagement. Wolters and Taylor [2012] suggested that there is a positive association between self-regulation and study engagement. Faisal et al. [2020] corroborated a positive relationship between cognitive and affective engagement and continued intention to use. Therefore, the present study chose study engagement as a mediator to understand how self-regulation affects positively the behavioral intention of learners to reuse.

The relationship between self-regulation, study engagement, and behavioral intention to reuse was also tested. Previous research has suggested that, when exploring the efficiency of e-learning, collaboration should also be considered [Papanikolaou and Boubouka, 2010; Cheng et al., 2011]. Peer collaboration is defined as online support by group members to reach a common learning goal [Cheng et al., 2011]. It has been uniformly regarded as a valuable resource that contributes to desired outcomes and should therefore be studied in most e-learning settings [Papanikolaou and Boubouka, 2010]. It may contribute to learners’ motivation toward reusing e-learning systems, and so, its moderating role concerning the effects of self-regulation on behavioral intention to reuse was examined.

The present study complements the e-learning literature in several ways. First, it fills a critical research gap in the research on the relationship between self-regulation and behavioral intention to reuse. Previous studies of behavioral intention to reuse focused primarily on the institutional environment beyond individual factors [Cheng et al., 2011]. Exploring the relationship between self-regulation and behavioral intention to reuse will help us have a more comprehensive understanding in terms of behavioral intention to reuse as self-regulation is an important indicator of effective learning and relates to outcomes such as academic motivation and self-efficacy [Lavasani et al., 2011].

Second, the present study contributes to the literature by showing why self-regulation affected behavioral intention to reuse. Although researchers have identified potential mediators, the motivational aspect [e.g., study engagement] of the influence of self-regulation has been neglected. Study engagement is worthy of investigation because motivation shapes learners’ reuse intention, and it is critical that educators understand how to enhance learners’ behavioral intention to reuse e-learning by encouraging them more. The other is that in the COVID-19 context, universities and schools should now always require that students give their full attention and energy to their e-learning tasks. Study engagement could be a possible mediator that transmits the influence of self-regulation on learners’ behavioral intention to reuse e-learning.

Third, we identified an important environment resource [i.e., peer collaboration] as a key boundary condition in enhancing the positive effect of self-regulation on study engagement and behavioral intention to reuse. Previous studies have demonstrated the positive effects of self-regulation but not of potential boundary conditions [e.g., Yeh et al., 2019]. Scholars should pay attention to how they might enhance the positive effects of self-regulation in e-learning. Our study wants to make a further step to identify a context factor in accelerating the positive effect of self-regulation. In particular, it introduced a moderated mediation model [Figure 1] positing study engagement as a mediator of the self-regulation’s effects on behavioral intention to reuse, with peer collaboration as a moderator of such effects.

FIGURE 1

Figure 1. The proposed model.

Theoretical Background and Hypotheses

Conservation of Resource Theory

As has been noted, the present study builds on the conservation of resource theory [Hobfoll, 1989] to understand the link between self-regulation and behavioral intention to reuse. Conservation of resource theory assumes that individuals tend to protect and retain the resources they value. The resource could be anything that people think is valuable [Hobfoll, 1989]. It suggests that when individuals lack resources, they tend to withdraw behavioral intention to minimize their loss. By contrast, individuals are interested in investing in more resources when such an investment generates returns. Because behavioral intention to reuse is considered a type of behavioral attitude toward acceptance and satisfaction [Chiu et al., 2005], which reflects a subjective expectation of how many resources learners can obtain by using e-learning systems, we hypothesized that self-regulation is positively related to behavioral intention to reuse.

Self-Regulation and Behavioral Intention to Reuse

Behavioral intention refers to the degree to which an individual intends to perform some specific future behavior [Davis and Warshaw, 1992]. In the context of e-learning, behavioral intention to reuse has been defined as the likelihood that learners will use an e-learning system again when it is made available to them [Budu et al., 2018]. As learners’ willingness to adopt an e-learning system determines the success of any e-learning system [Li et al., 2012; Almaiah, 2018], understanding the factors that influence behavioral intention to reuse has been the focus of numerous e-learning studies [Al-Emran and Teo, 2020; Zhang et al., 2020]. Previous research has identified several variables influencing learners’ decisions to use e-learning systems. These include perceived usefulness and perceived ease of use [Liaw, 2008]; self-efficacy and attitudes toward use [Amin et al., 2015]; the experience of e-learning [Mailizar et al., 2021]; and social influence [Liaw, 2008].

Self-regulation is an important factor in effective learning and is widely used to explain learning outcomes in e-learning research. Self-regulated behaviors include but are not limited to environment structuring, goal setting, time management, help-seeking, task strategies, and self-evaluation [Barnard et al., 2009]. According to the conservation of resource theory, the more learners perceive e-learning systems to be beneficial, the more they will use them. Because self-regulation enables learners to identify e-learning resources to achieve their goals [Wong et al., 2019], it can be assumed that it will increase their desire and belief to reuse them. Broadbent and Poon [2015] carried out a comprehensive analysis of self-regulation and concluded that self-regulation can help learners to achieve academic success in higher education settings. Various self-regulation outcomes have also been examined in previous studies [Panadero, 2017; van Alten et al., 2020a], for instance, satisfaction [van Alten et al., 2020b], learning performance [Zheng and Zhang, 2020], and academic achievement [Broadbent and Poon, 2015]. Based on the above literature, we proposed the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 1: Self-regulation is positively related to students’ behavioral intention to reuse e-learning systems.

The Mediating Role of Study Engagement

The concept of study engagement has received increased attention in the e-learning literature [Rodgers, 2008; Moubayed et al., 2020]. Study engagement is a psychological state of vigor, absorption, and dedication toward learning tasks [Lamborn et al., 1992] and reflects “the amount of effort dedicated to educational activities that bring out ideal performance” [Hu and Kuh, 2002, p. 555]. Individual resources are an essential component of an individual’s engagement in their studies [Ryan, 2005]. Previous literature has identified several resources that facilitate student engagement, such as interest in learning, belongingness [Hu and Kuh, 2002], deep learning, and relationship with others [Gunuc and Kuzu, 2015]. Also, study engagement is associated with achievement in the academy and psychological well-being [Deakin Crick and Goldspink, 2014]. Engagement in this study acts as a mediator that links self-regulation to behavioral intention to reuse. More explicitly, learners who can apply self-regulation strategies and skills will perceive a more positive state of passion and energy, which, in turn, leads to continuous use of e-learning systems.

The conservation of resource theory is a renowned paradigm for understanding study engagement [Gunuc and Kuzu, 2015]. According to this theory [Hobfoll, 1989], engagement consumes energy, so individuals need to replenish their resources to maintain a high level [Xu et al., 2019a,b]. The e-learning literature has also suggested that the e-learning environment is characterized by self-control, autonomy, and independence [Lynch and Dembo, 2004; Chiu et al., 2005]; engagement in learning heavily relies on a student’s ability to control, manage, and plan learning programs [Ally, 2004]. We assumed that self-regulation may help learners actively engage in their studies by adding their resources. First, self-regulation strategies could help learners acquire cognitive resources. For example, rehearsal and metacognitive strategies have been shown to be positively related to the acquisition and retention of knowledge [Yukselturk and Bulut, 2007]. Second, self-regulation enables learners to manage their emotions in the pursuit of studying goals, which provides emotional resources for student engagement. Studies have shown that strategies of regulating emotions, such as situation selection, situation modification, attentional deployment, and response modulation, are linked to learners’ positive emotions [Webster and Hadwin, 2015]. Therefore, it can be seen that self-regulation may facilitate student engagement.

Furthermore, study engagement can affect students’ behavioral intention to reuse e-learning systems. Engagement is characterized by dedication, absorption, and vigor. Dedication reflects individuals’ desire to invest effort in studying. Absorption refers to the state individuals enter when they are completely immersed in a task. Vigor emphasizes one’s excitement and interest in study [Schaufeli et al., 2002a; Axelson and Flick, 2010]. Study engagement involves a series of activities, including behavioral engagement [e.g., concentration], emotional engagement [e.g., belonging and excitement], and cognitive engagement [e.g., mental effort; Webster and Hadwin, 2015]. In terms of the conservation of resource theory, learners tend to reuse e-learning systems when they perceive them to have value. Close engagement will make learners more persistent in their e-learning. Previous research has suggested that study engagement leads to a higher return in terms of satisfaction [Webster and Hadwin, 2015]; academic performance [Carini et al., 2006]; student success; and retention [Wyatt, 2011]. In light of the above, we proposed the following hypotheses:

Hypothesis 2: Study engagement mediates the relationship between self-regulation and behavioral intention to reuse e-learning systems.

The Moderating Role of Peer Collaboration

Although we expected that self-regulation would positively influence study engagement, which in turn impacts behavioral intention to reuse, the conservation of resource theory [Hobfoll, 1989] indicates that environmental factors influence how learners manage and make use of self-regulation strategies and skills and obtain new resources. In e-learning settings, peer collaboration is regarded as a form of learner support where students are encouraged to exchange ideas with other students [Biasutti, 2011]. It is also an important influence on mental health [Kim et al., 2020]. Drawing on these insights, we proposed that peer collaboration presents an environmental resource that may strengthen the positive impact of self-regulation on study engagement.

Learners with high levels of peer collaboration tend to possess greater learning resources and are subsequently more capable of engaging with their studies. Scholars have suggested that peer collaboration facilitates both academic and social integration [Papanikolaou and Boubouka, 2010; Al-Abri et al., 2017]. Learners with high levels of peer collaboration are more likely than learners with low levels of peer collaboration to perceive their self-regulation strategies and skills as an effective way to acquire more resources. Logically, learners who employ self-regulation already have the effective learning ability to obtain resources with which to invest in study engagement. If learners also receive a high level of peer support, they will become more confident in participating in e-learning activities. Peer collaboration not only helps learners transfer information and ideas to improve their self-regulation but also helps them to cooperate, improve learning climates, and enhance learning satisfaction. As a result, the relationship between self-regulation and study engagement should be stronger for learners with high levels of peer collaboration. By contrast, learners with low levels of peer collaboration should be more vulnerable to insufficient learning-related resources and less capable of investing their resources to enhance the outcomes of self-regulation, thus decreasing the effectiveness of self-regulation for integrating personal and contextual resources. Accordingly, we proposed the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 3: Peer collaboration moderates the relationship between self-regulation and study engagement; this relationship is more positive when peer collaboration is high rather than low.

Given that peer collaboration moderates the relation between self-regulation and study engagement and considering that study engagement is positively associated with behavioral intention to reuse, it is logical to suggest that peer collaboration also moderates the strength of the mediating mechanism for study engagement in the relationship between self-regulation and behavioral intention to reuse, which presents a moderated mediation model [Edwards and Lambert, 2007]. As previously mentioned, a stronger relation between self-regulation and behavioral intention to reuse will appear among learners with high levels of peer collaboration. Hence, the indirect effect of self-regulation on behavioral intention to reuse via study engagement may be stronger for a learner who collaborates with their peers. In particular, when a student has a high level of peer collaboration, the indirect effect of self-regulation on behavioral intention to reuse should be stronger. However, when a student is lacking such a high level of collaboration, self-regulation is less influential in promoting study engagement; consequently, the indirect effect of self-regulation on behavioral intention to reuse should be weaker. In light of the above, we proposed the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 4: Peer collaboration moderates the mediating effect of study engagement on the relationship between self-regulation and behavioral intention to reuse such that the indirect effect of self-regulation on behavioral intention to reuse via study engagement is stronger for learners with a high level of peer collaboration than for learners with a low level of peer collaboration.

Materials and Methods

Respondents and Procedures

We drew our sample from one large university in central China. The e-learning programs were part of the students’ regular school curriculum. The questionnaires were distributed among undergraduates within the e-learning system. They were asked to complete a self-assessment of their self-regulation, study engagement, peer collaboration, and behavioral intention to reuse the e-learning system. All participants were assured that all data would be anonymous. They signed an electronic consent form agreeing to their participation in the survey.

With the help of their professors, 400 students who engaged in e-learning activities were invited to take part. From this sample, 379 valid samples were retained a response rate of 94.75%. The average age was 20.72years [SD=2.17]; 67.81% were male, and 32.19% were female. In terms of grades, 19.90% were freshmen, 33.25% were sophomores, 24.01% were juniors, 14.77% were seniors, and 8.97% were students in the fifth year. Their majors were as follows: 8.18%, humanities; 33.25%, economics and management; 15.57%, science; 54.35%, engineering; 1.85%, law; 3.96%, medicine; and 3.69%, education.

Measurement

Self-Regulation

Self-regulation was measured, using a 24-item scale from Barnard et al. [2009]. Example items include the following: goal setting – “I set standards for my assignments in online courses”; environment structuring – “I know where I can study most efficiently for online courses”; task strategies – “I work extra problems in my online courses in addition to the assigned ones to master the course content”; time management – “I allocate extra studying time for my online courses because I know it is time-demanding”; help-seeking – “I find someone who is knowledgeable in course content so that I can consult with him or her when I need help”; and self-evaluation – “I summarize my learning in online courses to examine my understanding of what I have learned.” Participants were asked to respond on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 [strongly disagree] to 7 [strongly agree] at Time 1. The internal consistency is α=0.96.

Study Engagement

We used a 17-item scale developed by Schaufeli et al. [2002b] to measure the study engagement of learners. It has three dimensions, including vigor, dedication, and absorption. Example items include the following: vigor – “When I study, I feel like I am bursting with energy”; dedication – “I am enthusiastic about my studies”; and absorption – “I feel happy when I am studying intensively.” Participants were asked to respond on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 [strongly disagree] to 7 [strongly agree]. The internal consistency of the study engagement scale is α=0.97.

Behavioral Intention to Reuse

We measured behavioral intention to reuse with Li et al. [2012] three-item scale. Example items include “Assuming that I had access to the e-learning system, I intend to reuse it.” Participants were asked to respond on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 [strongly disagree] to 7 [strongly agree]. The internal consistency of the scale is α=0.89.

Peer Collaboration

Peer collaboration was measure through a five-item scale developed by Zhang et al. [2014]. An example item is “My group members and I actively work together to help each other understand the learning task.” Participants were asked to respond on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 [strongly disagree] to 7 [strongly agree]. The internal consistency of the scale is α=0.94.

Control Variables

Socio-demographics were considered these variables as potential control variables. Demographic variables included age, gender [1=male, 2=female], grade [1=first year, 2=second year, 3=third year, 4=fourth year, 5=fifth year], and major [1=humanity, 2=economy and management, 3=science, 4=engineer, 5=law, 6=medicine, 7=education].

Results

Preliminary Analyses

First, the confirmatory factor analysis with Mplus7.0 was used to examine the distinctiveness of the latent variables. The results showed that a theorized four-factor model distinguishing between self-regulation, study engagement, behavioral intention to reuse, and peer collaboration was a better fit to the data [χ2=482.34, df=113, p

Chủ Đề