What does the physiological reactivity model examine?

Introduction

The neighborhoods we live in are important for our health. Independent of individual or family-level characteristics, neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage is associated with early mortality and worse physical and mental health1,2,3,4,5. Understanding the mechanisms underlying these associations is critical for improving population health and for testing theories concerning how broad social and contextual factors influence psychological and biological functioning. Neighborhoods may affect health over the life course through a variety of pathways including social services, health behaviors, physical or material exposures and social factors such as social support, social capital, and social disorder3,4,5,6,7.

Stress is also a central mechanism by which neighborhoods are theorized to influence health. Perceiving neighborhood contexts as stressful may induce negative emotion and biological stress responses3,4,5,7,8, which have widespread systemic effects that are thought to link adversity to health and mental health over time8,9,10,11,12. Indeed, neighborhood disadvantage is associated with indicators of chronic physiological stress, such as allostatic load9,13,14 and the baseline function and diurnal pattern of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis15,16. In addition, neighborhood conditions are associated with cortisol and blood pressure reactivity to stressful conditions, though the direction of effects varies and may depend on other factors, such as sex and race/ethnicity17,18,19,20. Neighborhood environments may not only affect acute stress and emotion, but also may affect chronic stress and emotional functioning due to accumulation of acute stress responses and the long-term toll of adapting to adverse neighborhood environments6,8,10,11. Over time, this adaptation may take the form of either habituation (i.e. reduced reactivity to neighborhood conditions over time) or sensitization (i.e. increased reactivity with repeated exposures)10,21,22, which have different implications for the processes linking neighborhood stressors and health8,10,11,22.

Despite the theoretical importance of stress mechanisms, to our knowledge there are no studies demonstrating that neighborhood environments themselves elicit acute emotional and stress responses, nor are there studies that are able to examine whether neighborhood conditions are associated with patterns of habituation or sensitization. In part, this may be due to methodological challenges. First, there are no standardized neighborhood-related stress response protocols, and thus most studies use stress reactivity tasks in the laboratory that are not related to neighborhood environments or experiences. Second, studies measuring stress and emotion in real neighborhood contexts cannot standardize and control exposures so that comparisons are equivalent across participants.

To address these issues, we developed a novel, virtual reality (VR) experimental model that approximates the experience of being in either a disadvantaged or affluent neighborhood. VR has been used to study highly stressful combat-related exposures23, stress reactivity24,25,26,27, social environment characteristics28,29,30, and habituation in exposure therapy31. As such, VR may also be suitable to test the hypothesis that exposure to simulated neighborhood conditions elicits immediate and differential emotional and stress responses. Moreover, acute VR exposures can be utilized in order to test hypotheses concerning habituation and sensitization. Here, prior stressful experiences may influence the response to VR neighborhood conditions, resulting in decreased responses consistent with habituation or increased responses consistent with sensitization.

We designed two VR neighborhoods to represent disadvantage and affluence (see Fig. 1 and Supplemental Videos S1, S2, showing sample blocks from each neighborhood), using the Unity3D game engine (https://unity3d.com) and the Experiments in Virtual Environments (EVE) computing framework32. Neighborhoods were differentiated by building and business types, signals of social and physical disorder (e.g., graffiti, litter), the level of upkeep or deterioration, green space, the presence of amenities and services (e.g., health services, community centers, parks), and other features such as security elements33,34,35. To represent different business types across neighborhoods, signs for business and services were designed and strategically placed on shopfronts and other buildings, such as those for grocery stores, restaurants, fast food, convenience stores, financial services, health care, real estate, lodging, clothing stores, and public services. Attention was also paid to the interaction between features in the neighborhoods, such as how the meaning of a park may change based on whether it is deteriorated or if there are signals such as broken liquor bottles36. The neighborhoods were populated by a set of human avatars that reflect the overall demographics of the United States (based on U.S. Census Bureau, Profile of General Population and Housing Characteristics: 2010), balanced to ensure that avatar type (e.g. professional and casual) was not biased by race/ethnicity or gender, and held constant across the two neighborhoods. In addition, each neighborhood was equipped with spatialized background city noise depicting traffic, construction, birds and human vocalizations. Sounds were equivalent across conditions (e.g. traffic, birds, water, human vocalizations), but the amount of each sound changed to remain congruent with the environment (e.g. more sounds of birds and water in well-kept parks).

Figure 1

What does the physiological reactivity model examine?

Virtual neighborhoods. Example scenes from virtual (a) affluent and (b) disadvantaged neighborhoods, along with a sample of the signs created for businesses and services in the neighborhoods. Note: Business names and logos are fictional creations and any correspondence with real businesses is unintentional and coincidental.

Full size image

In the present study, we employed a systematic social observation to verify if virtual neighborhoods were appraised as distinct representations of disadvantage and affluence. In addition, after navigating the neighborhoods, participants reported the emotions they felt while immersed in the neighborhood environment to determine if exposure to disadvantage elicited a more negative affective response than exposure to affluence. To determine whether disadvantaged neighborhood conditions elicited differential autonomic nervous system and cardiovascular stress responses, we measured systolic (SBP) and diastolic (DBP) blood pressure, non-specific skin conductance responses (NS.SCRs), and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) throughout exposure to different neighborhood conditions. Finally, we also tested the hypothesis that responses depend on prior experience. In particular, we sought to determine if prior stressful experiences, as indexed with parental education37,38, were associated with patterns of reactivity to neighborhood conditions that are consistent with habituation or sensitization. In particular, habituation would be evident if participants responded less to experimental neighborhoods that are congruent with their parental education level. In contrast, sensitization would be evident if participants exhibited greater responses to the VR neighborhoods congruent with their parental education level.

Materials and Methods

Participants and design

The study was conducted at ETH Zürich, and participants were recruited through the ETH Zürich Decision Science Laboratory online database (DeSciL, https://www.uast.uzh.ch). Participants were eligible for inclusion if they reported they were in good physical and mental health, 18–40 years old, English-speaking, with normal or corrected vision, and able to use their dominant hand for joystick manipulation and non-dominant hand for physiological measurements. Seventy-six participants were recruited, with seven excluded due to technical issues and one due to simulator sickness. Sixty-eight participants were thus randomly assigned to either the affluent or the disadvantaged virtual reality (VR) neighborhood (34 per condition). The majority of participants (98.5%) were graduate or undergraduate students, evenly distributed by sex, with a mean age of 22.7 years (range 18–31). In addition, 64.7 percent of participants had at least one parent with a college degree or higher. Table 1 reports the characteristics of the full sample and each experimental group and illustrates that there were no differences in baseline characteristics between experimental groups (all p ≥ 0.17). The study was approved by the ETH Zürich ethics commission (EK 2013-N-73) and performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. All participants provided written informed consent prior to the start of the experiment.

Table 1 Sample descriptive statistics and comparison across experimental conditions.

Full size table

Neighborhood protocol

Prior to neighborhood navigation, participants watched a 7-minute nature video of forest and water landscapes to establish a baseline for measurement of physiological reactivity. During neighborhood navigation, participants followed a route marked by intermittent arrows with some limited freedom to explore the environment. Figure 2a shows the paths walked throughout each neighborhood. To prompt visual scanning and promote exploration of the environment, participants were also asked to collect a series of tokens that were strategically placed around the neighborhoods. After neighborhood immersion, participants completed a systematic social observation to quantify and validate their perception of the neighborhood34,39. On average it took 20.5 minutes (SD = 3.6) to navigate the virtual environment, and there were no differences in the percentage of tokens collected for each neighborhood, t(50.99) = 1.77, p = 0.081.

Figure 2

What does the physiological reactivity model examine?

Neighborhood environments: Paths and Perceptions. (a) Paths navigated through the neighborhood environments in a virtual city (SA/SD represent the start point for the affluent and disadvantaged routes, respectively). Participants followed the same route with some freedom to explore. Thicker lines represent the most popular paths, while thinner lines represent less common paths some participants explored. (b,c) Perceptions of neighborhood characteristics for each experimental condition from the SSO ratings39. For each SSO measure, bars represent (b) mean percentage of items endorsed on each scale, and (c) mean ratings, with standard errors. Indices of statistical significance are from the Mann-Whitney U test.

Full size image

Adult and child avatars were used to enhance the neighborhood social environment. We used Adobe Fuse Character Creator (https://www.adobe.com/products/fuse.html) to create 152 adult avatars, animated with Mixamo’s 3D software (https://www.mixamo.com). Unity’s navigation mesh (NavMesh) feature was employed to define the walkable surfaces, and avatars were spawned from different locations and programmed to walk to a series of destinations. Avatars were also placed at static locations, such as standing on corners or sitting at café tables, with some conversing with each other. The avatars did not interact with participants or respond to attempts at social engagement. In case of collision, the avatars were programmed to apologize with a gesture.

VR Setup

The VR system included a 55″ ultra-high definition television display (Samsung UE55JU6470, 3840 × 2160 pixels) for desktop presentation and a high-end gaming computer (Dell Alienware Area 51 Base with a 3.8 GHz overclocked i7–5820K processor, dual NVIDIA GeForce GTX 1080 video cards and 32 GB of SDRAM) running Windows 10. Participants were seated facing the television display at a distance of 1.6 meters, used a joystick (Mad Catz V.1 Stick) to navigate, a wireless keyboard and mouse to answer questionnaires, and wore headphones capable of producing spatialized sounds (Logitech G633 Artemis Spectrum with 7.1 Surround).

Measures

Participant characteristics

We used the MacArthur Network Sociodemographic Questionnaire (http://www.macses.ucsf.edu), adapted for use in Switzerland, to collect sociodemographic information. Participants were from 19 different countries across five continents, though the majority were originally from Europe (73.5%).

Participants also rated the quality and safety of their current neighborhood40. In terms of overall neighborhood ratings, 48.5% participants rated their neighborhood as “excellent”, 47.1% rated it as “good”, and only 4.4% of participants rated their neighborhood as “fair”. With respect to safety, 51.5% of participants rated their neighborhood as “extremely safe”, while the other 48.5% rated it as “quite safe”.

Parental educational level

The maximum level of education achieved by either parent was used as a socioeconomic indicator of prior stressful experiences37,38. Parental education was chosen as it is more stable and more likely to be known by participants compared to other socioeconomic indicators from childhood such as income. College degree was chosen (1 = college degree or above, 0 = below a college degree), a priori, as it is an important gateway to knowledge, social resources and prestige, and thus is a good retrospective marker of family socioeconomic status (SES).

Systematic social observation (SSO)

Participants completed a modified version of the SSO i-Tour34,39 to measure appraisal of the neighborhoods. The SSO i-Tour is designed for use in a digital environment (Google Maps/Street View) and generates multiple scales34. The physical disorder scale is a count of features such as graffiti, garbage or litter on street (range 0–5, 5 = 100% of items endorsed). The physical decay scale is a count of the ratings of poor or badly deteriorated conditions across streets, sidewalks, etc. (range 0–4). A street safety scale sums reports of the presence of safety measures, such as crosswalks, speed limit signs, and bike lanes (range 0–6). Neighborhood dangerousness/safety is the average rating of how safe the neighborhood was overall and how safe they would feel walking there at night (range 0–4). Green space was indicated by estimates of how much of the neighborhood had trees (3 = 75% or more; 2 = 50–74%, 1 = 1–49%, and 0 = None). Finally, participants appraised the neighborhood’s SES, whether it was best characterized as: wealthy/prosperous, comfortably off, of moderate means, or poor. Descriptive results are presented in Fig. 2b,c, and Supplementary Table S1. There were no differences in participant ratings of the weather χ2 (2, N = 68) = 0.90, p = 0.64, or the time of day χ2(1, N = 68) = 1.34, p = 0.25, which were held constant across neighborhoods, suggesting that differences in neighborhood perceptions are unlikely to be due to general differences in reporting.

Gaming experience and simulator sickness

To rule out differences in response to VR associated with gaming experience, total hours of weekly video game usage were assessed with a video game playing questionnaire41. In addition, the total severity score from the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire42 was used to control for differences in emotional and physiological responses that could be due to the VR protocol.

Emotional responses

After completing the experiment, participants were asked how they felt while in the neighborhood. The Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM)43 measured affect (from happy to unhappy) and alertness/arousal (from excited – calm) on a dimensional scale. In addition, participants reported how strongly they felt nine emotions (amusement, anger, contentment/happiness, compassion, disgust, enthusiasm/excitement, fear, sadness, and surprise, 0–8 scale)44. Supplementary Table S2 presents the descriptive statistics and correlations among responses. Composite measures were created for positive (amusement, contentment/happiness, enthusiasm/excitement) and negative emotions (anger, disgust, fear, sadness) by averaging the z-scores for individual emotions. Analyses were repeated utilizing two factors, corresponding to positive and negative emotion, using both promax and varimax rotations. Findings follow the same pattern as those reported using composites (analyses available upon request). Compassion and surprise were examined individually in exploratory analyses because they positively correlated with individual measures of both positive and negative emotions.

Physiological reactivity

Blood pressure, electrodermal activity and respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) were utilized as indices of autonomic nervous system and cardiovascular stress responses45,46,47.

Blood pressure: Systolic (SBP) and diastolic (DBP) blood pressure were recorded at 3-minute intervals (Phillips SureSigns VS2 + ). One participant was omitted from analyses for DBP as multiple readings were greater than 3 SD above the mean. Three measures of SBP were not recorded by the system. Consequently SBP analyses included 462 measures for 68 participants and DBP analyses included 458 observations for 67 participants.

Electrodermal activity (EDA): EDA was collected with two finger electrodes (MLT118F, ADInstruments) attached to the middle segment of the index and ring finger. Electrodes were connected to the PowerLab 8/35 recording device through a FE116 GSRAmp signal amplifier (ADInstruments). Data were recorded at a sample rate of 1000 Hz using LabChart version 8.1.4 (ADInstruments). Temperature (M = 22.8 °C (range 21.5–24) and humidity (M = 35.9%, range 31–51) in the testing room were kept within the optimal range48,49, with no differences by experimental condition for temperature, t(66) = −1.13, p = 0.26, or humidity, t(66) = 0.98, p = 0.33.

Given the duration of neighborhood exposure and the multiplicity of potentially evocative stimuli, it was hypothesized that neighborhood exposure would result in heightened reactivity overall, or an increased number of responses to an unpredictable set of stimuli. Consequently, analyses focused on non-specific skin conductance responses (NS.SCR’s). NS.SCR’s are discrete increases in skin conductance that are not linked to a specific stimulus48 and defined at a threshold 0.01 µS for each response window. Total NS.SCR’s were calculated per minute, with 1,361 observations across participants.

Matlab (R2017a, MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) was used to export events from LabChart and to separate them into 1-minute interval windows. Data were eliminated for intervals that were less than one-minute. Data were processed using Ledalab software version 3.4.9 (www.ledalab.de), and were first down sampled to 10 Hz and visually inspected for artifacts, none of which were found. We used continuous decomposition analysis (CDA)50 to extract NS.SCR’s. Values were optimized two times to improve the estimation of the parameters of the impulse response function.

Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA): Electrocardiography (ECG) was collected using the ADInstruments Powerlab 8/35 Data Acquisition System with a Bio Amp (FE132, ADInstruments) and three disposable electrodes (MLA1010, ADInstruments) placed on the second intercostal space below the middle of the clavicle on the right (RA) and left (LA) side of the chest accordingly. A third electrode was attached below the 9th left rib (LL)51. ECG was sampled at a rate of 1000 Hz, with LabChart data acquisition software (LabChart v8.1.4, ADInstruments). ECG data were analyzed with Kubios HRV software, version 3.152. The time series of interbeat intervals was generated for each participant and visually inspected for correct beat identification. Kubios also automatically identified and corrected artifacts – these corrections were visually inspected and impacted 0.71% of overall data points and no more than 3.12% of beats for any individual participant. The signal was processed using the very low threshold filter setting (0.45 sec) and the smooth priors filter for detrending (λ = 500, fc = 0.035 Hz). Data were analyzed in one-minute epochs, using a Fast Fourier transform focusing on power in the high frequency range (0.15–0.4 Hz) reflective of parasympathetic nervous system activity53,54. Data were eliminated for epochs that were less than one-minute or for which ECG derived respiration estimates indicated that respiration frequency was out of the high frequency band. Absolute values of high frequency power were natural log transformed, generating a measure of RSA for each minute of the protocol. The RSA analysis thus consists of 1,325 observations from 67 participants across the protocol.

Procedures

The experiment was conducted in a curtained, private area at the NeuroLab at ETH Zürich. Participants were asked to abstain from caffeine, tobacco, alcohol and exercise for at least three hours before the experiment. Participants used a joystick to navigate, which they were trained to use, and practiced in a virtual maze to ensure they were adept with the control interface and task procedures and had time to recover from any initial, general response to the VR environment (see Supplementary Fig. S1 for full experimental protocol). The experiment was deployed within EVE (Experiments in Virtual Environments)32,51, a comprehensive computing framework for the presentation of virtual environments and the collection of behavioral and physiological data (available at https://cog-ethz.github.io/EVE/).

Data analysis

To determine if there were differences in perception of the affluent and disadvantaged neighborhoods, we utilized the Mann-Whitney U test, given violations of assumptions for normal distribution (Fig. 2b,c, and Supplementary Table S1). These analyses, and all regression models, were conducted using IBM SPSS versions 24 and 25. All statistical tests were two-sided.

We examined the main effect of neighborhood condition on emotion and physiological responses to determine if there was an acute effect. Moderation analyses were used to test the hypothesis that responses depend on prior experience, and if they are consistent with habituation or sensitization, by examining the interaction between parental education and VR neighborhood condition. We employed multiple linear regression models for analyses of emotional responses.

For physiological measures, we used linear mixed-effects modeling in SAS 9.4 to account for repeated measures. Models were estimated using restricted maximum likelihood and an unstructured covariance structure. To determine the best fitting model, we started with an unconditional model and then added fixed and random effects for linear and quadratic terms for time during the experiment, comparing models utilizing-2 Log-likelihood and the Akaike and Bayes Information Criterion (AIC/BIC). For models of SBP and DBP, the best fitting model included both a random intercept and fixed effect for a linear index of time, in minutes, during the experiment. For NS.SCRs and RSA, the best fitting and most parsimonious model included a random intercept and both a random and fixed effect for the linear index of time. Due to inconsistency across fit criteria for NS.SCRs, we also examined models that added random and fixed effects for a quadratic index of time. Results were broadly equivalent to models without quadratic terms, and thus we chose the most parsimonious model. Baseline estimates of SBP, DBP, NS.SCR, and RSA correspond to the average of measurements during the 7-minute nature video. This baseline measure was subtracted from each data point during neighborhood exposures such that each repeated measure represents reactivity in comparison to baseline. Consequently main effects and interactions in the model can be interpreted as predicting overall blood pressure, RSA, or NS.SCR reactivity to the task. We also conducted exploratory analyses to examine potential differences in the growth rate of reactivity between experimental conditions and based on parental education. As no interactions with time were significant (all p ≥ 0.11), they were not included in models. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d, with the interaction effect size being the difference between the effect size for the experimental condition at each level of parental education.

Finally, we ran models controlling for a priori covariates to ensure effects were specific (see Supplementary Tables S3–S8). These models were run individually due to the study sample size, which precludes including all covariates in a single model. Covariates included age, sex, current education level (Graduate Student = 1, Undergraduate or Not in School = 0), hours of video games played, national origin (Swiss National versus other), smoking status, subjective perceptions of current neighborhood safety and quality, simulator sickness, as well as the amount time navigating the neighborhoods, which was longer for the affluent condition, t(66) = 3.48, p = 0.001).

Results

Participants perceived the neighborhoods to be distinct and in concert with the type of neighborhood differences observed with systematic social observations34,39 (see Fig. 2b,c and Supplementary Table S1). Participants that navigated the disadvantaged neighborhood reported significantly higher levels of physical disorder, Mann-Whitney U-test, U(68) = 32, p < 0.001, and decay, U(68) = 51, p < 0.001, compared to those that navigated the affluent neighborhood. Ratings of street safety, U(68) = 324.5, p = 0.001, appraisal of trees in the neighborhood, U(68) = 189.5, p < 0.001, and overall appraisal of safety, U(68) = 0.0, p < 0.001, were rated higher in the affluent neighborhood than in the disadvantaged neighborhood. In addition, the overall perceived socioeconomic status (SES) was rated significantly higher in the affluent neighborhood, U(68) = 0.0, p < 0.001. Indeed, all participants rated the affluent neighborhood as one of “wealth/prosperity” or “comfortably off”, and the disadvantaged neighborhood as one of “moderate means” or “poor”.

Exposure to the disadvantaged neighborhood, compared to the affluent neighborhood, was associated with higher composite scores of negative emotion, β = −0.68, t(66) = −7.46, p < 0.001, lower composite scores of positive emotion, β = 0.41, t(66) = 3.65, p = 0.001, as well as less happiness on a dimensional scale of affect43, β = 0.55, t(66) = 5.38, p < 0.001 (see Fig. 3 and Supplementary Tables S3–S5). Moreover, exposure to the disadvantaged neighborhood was associated with greater compassion during the task, β = −0.27, t(66) = −2.29, p = 0.025, (see Supplementary Table S6) though it was not associated with arousal, β = −0.21, t(66) = −1.77, p = 0.082, or surprise, β = −0.04, t(66) = −0.29, p = 0.77 (see Fig. 3). These associations were independent of age, sex, current education level, video game usage, national origin, smoking status, ratings or comparisons of current neighborhood safety and quality, simulator sickness, and time navigating the city (Supplementary Tables S3–S6). However, none of the interactions between neighborhood condition and parental education were significant (all p ≥ 0.07; see Supplementary Tables S3–S6) and thus there was no evidence of either habituation or sensitization of emotional responses to neighborhood conditions.

Figure 3

What does the physiological reactivity model examine?

Emotional responses. Panels illustrate emotional responses to different neighborhood conditions for (a) composite measures of positive and negative emotion; (b) compassion and surprise; and (c) dimensional measures of affect and arousal. Bars represent mean composite scores, or mean scores for individual measures, with standard errors. Indices of statistical significance are from linear regression models.

Full size image

Mixed effects models accounting for repeated measures indicated that, compared to baseline, there were no main effects of neighborhood condition for overall systolic blood pressure (SBP) reactivity, B = −0.04, t(393) = −0.06, p = 0.95, d = −0.002, diastolic blood pressure (DBP) reactivity, B = 0.38, t(390) = 0.57, p = 0.57 d = −0.13, electrodermal reactivity [non-specific skin conductance responses (NS.SCR)], B = −0.27, t(1225) = −0.15, p = 0.88, d = 0.10, or respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) reactivity, B = −0.01, t(1191) = −0.08, p = 0.93, d = 0.05. However, there was an interaction between VR condition and parental education for SBP reactivity, B = −2.98, t(393) = −2.04, p = 0.043, d = 0.76, and electrodermal (NS.SCR) reactivity, B = −8.44, t(1225) = −2.28, p = 0.023, d = 0.61, indicative of moderation, but not for DBP reactivity, B = −1.69, t(390) = −1.18, p = 0.24, d = 0.39, or for RSA reactivity, B = −0.01, t(1191) = −0.03, p = 0.97, d = 0.007. For SBP this interaction remains significant when controlling for all covariates except for current education level and current neighborhood quality in comparison to others in the area (Supplementary Table S7). For NS.SCRs, this interaction remained significant when controlling for all covariates (Supplementary Table S8). Consequently, there is evidence that SBP and electrodermal reactivity to neighborhood conditions depend on parental education level.

As illustrated in Fig. 4, this interaction indicates that those whose parents had less than a college degree exhibited greater SBP reactivity (d = 0.55) and increases in NS.SCRs (d = 0.49) to the disadvantaged neighborhood, while those whose parents had a college degree or above were more reactive to the affluent neighborhood (SBP, d = −0.21; NS.SCRs, d = −0.12). This pattern is consistent with sensitization, rather than habituation, as there was increased reactivity for participants in the neighborhood condition that was congruent with their parental SES.

Figure 4

What does the physiological reactivity model examine?

Physiological responses to VR neighborhoods by parental education level. Bars represent mean change compared to baseline, based on the raw data, for all repeated measures across the entire experiment (with standard errors). Indices of statistical significance are for the interaction between experimental condition and parental education, from mixed model analyses.

Full size image

Discussion

This study provides the first support for the hypothesis that acute exposure to different neighborhood environments elicits differences in stress and emotional reactivity. Participant observations of the virtual environments indicated that they were perceived distinctly and as characterizing a more affluent versus disadvantaged neighborhood33,34,35. These results suggest that the virtual neighborhoods are a valid experimental model for use in studying the influence of neighborhood environments in a lab environment.

With respect to emotion, neighborhood disadvantage elicited more negative emotion and less positive emotion, with no evidence of habituation or sensitization. Overall, this is consistent with the role of emotion as a mediator of neighborhood effects on health and mental health, especially for neighborhood differences in mood disorder symptoms1,3,4,5,7. In particular, it suggests that acute exposure to more disadvantaged neighborhoods is associated with positive and negative emotion and, over time, these experiences may add up to influence mood and intersect with other life stressors. In addition to contributing to mood disorders, subclinical negative and positive emotional effects may contribute to differences in physical health via physiological or behavioral mechanisms55,56,57. However, such interpretations require caution and replication across additional, diverse samples. Moreover, the fact that exposure to disadvantage also elicited greater compassion suggests that these effects may be far more complex, and neighborhood exposures may also have potentially positive consequences.

In contrast, we did not find that neighborhood conditions elicit physiological stress or emotional responses by themselves, but instead that patterns of responding were dependent on prior experience. In particular, lower parental education was associated with greater reactivity to disadvantaged neighborhoods while higher parental education was associated with greater reactivity to affluent neighborhoods, though the effect in the affluent neighborhood was smaller in magnitude. These results are consistent with a pattern of sensitization to prior experience, in which continued exposure to stressors do not lead to habituation but leads instead to greater reactivity10,11,22. These findings also suggest that greater frequency or amplitude of stress responses may be part of the link between neighborhood stressors and health10, and are consistent with studies that have indicated that neighborhood disadvantage is associated with greater allostatic load9,13,14. Consequently, there is some evidence that, in a manner dependent on prior experience, neighborhood environments can elicit stress responses that may potentially contribute to differences in health.

In addition, we found different patterns across measures of physiological reactivity and, in particular, evidence of sensitization processes for blood pressure and electrodermal activity but not for respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA). RSA is a marker of parasympathetic nervous system activity and has been conceptualized as a peripheral biomarker of emotion regulation and the regulation of cardiac responses to environmental signals58,59,60. This raises the possibility that sensitization processes are specific to stress and arousal but do not encompass processes related to the regulation of emotional responses to neighborhood conditions. Additional studies are needed to confirm such speculation, and must include other measures such as HPA-axis reactivity, to determine the degree to which neighborhood influences on stress reactivity and sensitization-related processes are system-specific or general in nature.

Previous research has already shown that VR protocols can be successful in eliciting social stress. Studies utilizing a VR adaptation the Trier Social Stress Test24,25,26,27 have revealed that VR-based social stress can elicit systematic differences in physiological reactivity, some of which are comparable to the same stressor in vivo. Similarly, a VR protocol has been used to examine the effect of social stressors in a virtual bar, conceptualized as differences in avatar density, hostile facial expressions, and their degree of congruence with the race/ethnicity of the participant28,29,30. In case-control studies of psychosis liability, variation in these stressors have been associated with subjective distress, both for main effects30 and as moderated by prior trauma,29 and a small pilot study found some differences related to skin conductance28. Although the sample and stressor type differ, these studies also converge to suggest that VR can be feasibly deployed to study the influence of broader social and contextual environment on stress and emotion. Nonetheless, the current study is the first to examine the variation in broader, macro-level neighborhood environments in VR as a stressor.

Our study also points to the importance of considering which neighborhood features or characteristics (e.g. green space, nature, social and physical disorder) are most important for stress and emotion3,4,5,6,7. Some of these features are currently being studied as part of a growing literature that typically reports positive effects of short-term exposure to more natural environments on stress and emotion61,62,63,64. However, this strand of research has yet to examine the effects of different neighborhood environments or interactions between nature and neighborhood environments. Congruent with our findings, these studies raise the possibility that differences in stress and emotion may, in fact, account for the role of specific neighborhood features such as nature, or disorder, on health. As such, there may be an overlap or intersection between stress and other hypothesized mechanisms underlying neighborhood effects on health. Future studies should aim to systematically decompose complex VR neighborhood environments into constituent features to determine which, if any, are of greatest salience for stress and emotion.

The inferences that can be drawn concerning sensitization and habituation from this study are limited, in part, by the use of parental education as a retrospective index of prior stressful experiences. Parental education, in this study, was used as a reliable retrospective measure of family socioeconomic status (SES) that captures variation in social prestige, resources, and stressful experiences both within the family and in the family’s broader context37,38,65,66,67. Despite its utility, this marker does not assess psychosocial or physiological stress directly nor does it allow for investigation of stressor specificity, or which aspects of experience are most salient for habituation or sensitization. As such, a categorical indicator such as parental education may underestimate the degree to which prior stressful experiences moderate responses to the VR neighborhood environments. Parental education also does not specifically assess both objective and subjective lifecourse neighborhood experiences, which would provide the most specific test of sensitization and habituation processes related to neighborhood experiences themselves. These results, therefore, speak more to sensitization to prior experience more generally, rather than in terms of chronic neighborhood exposures. Future studies should utilize both measures of psychosocial and physiological stressors as well as lifecourse neighborhood experiences in order to more robustly examine potential habituation and sensitization-related processes.

Given the nature of the sample recruited, it is also possible that some of our findings are not generalizable. On the whole, participants were of higher family SES overall, well-educated, and students that currently live in good neighborhoods in an affluent region in Zurich. This range of experiences may influence how the VR neighborhoods are perceived, given that such perceptions are likely anchored in prior experience. It is also possible that the limited diversity of the sample, particularly in terms of SES and neighborhood exposures, may lead to an underestimation of effects compared to a broader, more diverse sample with more extensive experience with a range of neighborhood types. In order to make more generalizable inferences concerning neighborhood influences on stress, emotion and patterns of sensitization and habituation, it is critical that future studies be conducted with more diverse samples in additional geographic regions.

Beyond the generalizability of the current findings, examining consistency or variation of these patterns across different populations is important for testing more nuanced hypotheses concerning the influence of chronic stress and chronic neighborhood exposures, as this interaction suggests that autonomic and cardiovascular reactivity is most illuminating at the intersection between prior experience and acute exposure to neighborhood conditions. Consequently, in addition to studying more diverse samples, it is also important for such future work to examine how patterns of sensitization might emerge longitudinally, and which prior life course exposures, particularly at the neighborhood level, may account for these patterns. Prior experience may create physiological changes that drive these sensitization processes, or it may be that prior experience alters both the meaning or appraisal of potential stressors or promotes increased knowledge of and vigilance to the salient challenges in one’s typical environment. Only studies with more diverse samples with more complete characterization of lifecourse processes will be able to address such issues.

Despite limitations, results of this study support the validity and feasibility of utilizing VR as an experimental model to study some aspects of acute neighborhood influences on stress and emotion and to test hypotheses concerning how such effects differ based on prior experience. Moreover, these results provide support for the theoretical proposition that neighborhood contexts elicit different patterns of emotion and stress that are relevant for disparities in health and development.

References

  1. Ludwig, J. et al. Neighborhood Effects on the Long-Term Well-Being of Low-Income Adults. Science 337, 1505–1510 (2012).

    ADS  CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Chetty, R., Hendren, N. & Katz, L. F. The effects of exposure to better neighborhoods on children: New evidence from the Moving to Opportunity Experiment. Am. Econ. Rev. 106, 855–902 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Diez-Roux, A. V. & Mair, C. Neighborhoods and Health. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1186, 125–145 (2010).

    ADS  Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Robert, S. A. Socioeconomic position and health: The independent contribution of community socioeconomic context. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 25, 489–516 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Leventhal, T. & Brooks-Gunn, J. The neighborhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychol. Bull. 126, 309–337 (2000).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Browning, C. R., Cagney, K. A. & Boettner, B. Neighborhood, place and the life course. in Handbook of the Life Course (eds Shanahan, M. J., Mortimer, J. T. & Kirkpatrick Johnson, M.) II, 597–620 (Springer International Publishing, 2016).

  7. Ross, C. E. & Mirowsky, J. Neighborhood Disadvantage, Disorder, and Health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 42, 258–276 (2001).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Miller, G. E., Chen, E. & Parker, K. J. Psychological stress in childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging: Moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechanisms. Psychol. Bull. 137, 959–997 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Brody, G. H., Lei, M.-K., Chen, E. & Miller, G. E. Neighborhood poverty and allostatic load in African-American youth. Pediatrics 124, e1362–e1368 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. McEwen, B. S. Protective and Damaging Effects of Stress Mediators. N. Engl. J. Med. 338, 171–179 (1998).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Ellis, B. J. & Boyce, W. T. Biological sensitivity to context. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 183–187 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Taylor, S. E., Repetti, R. L. & Seeman, T. HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY: What is an Unhealthy Environment and How Does It Get Under the Skin? Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48, 411–447 (1997).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Robinette, J. W., Charles, S. T., Almeida, D. M. & Gruenewald, T. L. Neighborhood features and physiological risk: An examination of allostatic load. Health Place 41, 110–118 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Theall, K. P., Drury, S. S. & Shirtcliff, E. A. Cumulative Neighborhood Risk of Psychosocial Stress and Allostatic Load in Adolescents. Am. J. Epidemiol. 176, S164–S174 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Karb, R. A., Elliott, M. R., Dowd, J. B. & Morenoff, J. D. Neighborhood-level stressors, social support, and diurnal patterns of cortisol: the Chicago Community Adult Health Study. Soc. Sci. Med. 75, 1038–1047 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hajat, A. et al. Examining the cross-sectional and longitudinal association between diurnal cortisol and neighborhood characteristics: Evidence from the multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Health Place 34, 199–206 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Jackson, R. W., Treiber, F. A., Turner, J. R., Davis, H. & Strong, W. B. Effects of race, sex, and socioeconomic status upon cardiovascular stress responsivity and recovery in youth. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 31, 111–119 (1999).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Hackman, D. A., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Hurt, H. & Farah, M. J. Neighborhood disadvantage and adolescent stress reactivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6 (2012).

  19. Theall, K. P., Shirtcliff, E. A., Dismukes, A. R., Wallace, M. & Drury, S. S. Association between neighborhood violence and biological stress in children. JAMA Pediatr. 171, 53–60 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Barrington, W. E. et al. Socioeconomic deprivation, perceived neighborhood factors, and cortisol responses to induced stress among healthy adults. Health Place 27, 120–126 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Grissom, N. & Bhatnagar, S. Habituation to repeated stress: get used to it. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 92, 215–224 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Harkness, K. L., Hayden, E. P. & Lopez-Duran, N. L. Stress Sensitivity and Stress Sensitization in Psychopathology: An Introduction to the Special Section. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 124, 1–3 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Costanzo, M. E. et al. Psychophysiological response to virtual reality and subthreshold posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in recently deployed military. Psychosom. Med. 76, 670–677 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Fallon, M. A., Careaga, J. S., Sbarra, D. A. & O’Connor, M.-F. Utility of a virtual Trier Social Stress Test: Initial findings and benchmarking comparisons. Psychosom. Med. 78, 835–840 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Hawn, S. E., Paul, L., Thomas, S., Miller, S. & Amstadter, A. B. Stress reactivity to an electronic version of the Trier Social Stress Test: a pilot study. Front. Psychol. 6 (2015).

  26. Kothgassner, O. D. et al. Salivary cortisol and cardiovascular reactivity to a public speaking task in a virtual and real-life environment. Comput. Hum. Behav. 62, 134–135 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Zimmer, P., Buttlar, B., Halbeisen, G., Walther, E. & Domes, G. Virtually stressed? A refined virtual reality adaptation of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) induces robust endocrine responses. Psychoneuroendocrinology 101, 186–192 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Veling, W., Brinkman, W.-P., Dorrestijn, E. & van der Gaag, M. Virtual Reality Experiments Linking Social Environment and Psychosis: A Pilot Study. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 17, 191–195 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Veling, W., Counotte, J., Pot-Kolder, R., van Os, J. & van der Gaag, M. Childhood trauma, psychosis liability and social stress reactivity: a virtual reality study. Psychol. Med. 46, 3339–3348 (2016).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Veling, W., Counotte, J., van der Gaag, M., Pot-Kolder, R. & van Os, J. Environmental Social Stress, Paranoia and Psychosis Liability: A Virtual Reality Study. Schizophr. Bull. 42, 1363–1371 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Rothbaum, B. O., Rizzo, A. “Skip” & Difede, J. Virtual reality exposure therapy for combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1208, 126–132 (2010).

    ADS  Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Grübel, J. et al. EVE: A Framework for Experiments in Virtual Environments. In Spatial Cognition X (eds Barkowsky, T., Burte, H., Hölscher, C. & Schultheis, H.) 159–176 (Springer International Publishing, 2017).

  33. Jones, M., Pebley, A. R. & Sastry, N. Eyes on the block: Measuring urban physical disorder through in-person observation. Soc. Sci. Res. 40, 523–537 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Odgers, C. L., Caspi, A., Bates, C. J., Sampson, R. J. & Moffitt, T. E. Systematic social observation of children’s neighborhoods using Google Street View: a reliable and cost-effective method. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 1009–1017 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Sampson, R. J. & Raudenbush, S. W. Systematic social observation of public spaces: A new look at disorder in urban neighborhoods. Am. J. Sociol. 105, 603–651 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Schuch, J. C. et al. Por Nuestros Ojos: Understanding social determinants of health through the eyes of youth. Prog. Community Health Partnersh. Res. Educ. Action 8, 197–205 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Evans, G. W. The environment of childhood poverty. Am. Psychol. 59, 77–92 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Goodman, E., McEwen, B. S., Dolan, L. M., Schafer-Kalkhoff, T. & Adler, N. E. Social disadvantage and adolescent stress. J. Adolesc. Health 37, 484–492 (2005).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Odgers, C. L., Bates, C. J., Caspi, A., Sampson, R. J. & Moffitt, T. E. Systematic social observation inventory – tally of observations in urban regions (SSO i-Tour). (Adaptlab publications, 2009).

  40. Echeverria, S. E., Diez-Roux, A. V. & Link, B. G. Reliability of self-reported neighborhood characteristics. J. Urban Health 81, 682–701 (2004).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Green, C. S. et al. Playing some video games but not others is related to cognitive abilities: A critique of Unsworth et al. (2015). Psychol. Sci. 28, 679–682 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Kennedy, R. S., Lane, N. E., Berbaum, K. S. & Lilienthal, M. G. Simulator sickness questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 3, 203–220 (1993).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Bradley, M. M. & Lang, P. J. Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 25, 49–59 (1994).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Haase, C. M., Seider, B. H., Shiota, M. N. & Levenson, R. W. Anger and sadness in response to an emotionally neutral film: evidence for age-specific associations with well-being. Psychol. Aging 27, 305–317 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Mauss, I. B. & Robinson, M. D. Measures of emotion: A review. Cogn. Emot. 23, 209–237 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Kamarck, T. W. & Lovallo, W. R. Cardiovascular reactivity to psychological challenge: Conceptual and measurement considerations. Psychosom. Med. 65, 9–21 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Stern, R. M., Ray, W. J. & Quigley, K. S. Psychophysiological recording. (Oxford University Press, 2001).

  48. Society for Psychophysiological Research Ad Hoc Committee on Electrodermal Measures. Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology 49, 1017–1034 (2012).

  49. Braithwaite, J. J., Watson, D. G., Jones, R. & Rowe, M. A guide for analysing electrodermal activity (EDA) and skin conductance responses (SCRs) for psychophysiological experiments via the MP36R and AcqKnowledge software (Revised Version 2.0). (Technical Report: Selective Attention & Awareness Laboratory (SAAL). Behavioural Brain Sciences Centre, University of Birmingham, UK., 2015).

  50. Benedek, M. & Kaernbach, C. A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. J. Neurosci. Methods 190, 80–91 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Weibel, R. P. et al. Virtual Reality Experiments with Physiological Measures. J. Vis. Exp. 138, e58318 (2018).

    Google Scholar 

  52. Tarvainen, M. P., Niskanen, J.-P., Lipponen, J. A., Ranta-aho, P. O. & Karjalainen, P. A. Kubios HRV – Heart rate variability analysis software. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 113, 210–220 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Berntson, G. G. et al. Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology 34, 623–648 (1997).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Task Force of the European Society for Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation 93, 1043–1065 (1996).

  55. Gallo, L. C. & Matthews, K. A. Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emotions play a role? Psychol. Bull. 129, 10–51 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Cohen, S. & Pressman, S. D. Positive Affect and Health. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 122–125 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. DeSteno, D., Gross, J. J. & Kubzansky, L. Affective science and health: The importance of emotion and emotion regulation. Health Psychol. 32, 474–486 (2013).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Porges, S. W. The polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol. 74, 116–143 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Thayer, J. F. & Lane, R. D. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J. Affect. Disord. 61, 201–216 (2000).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Beauchaine, T. P. Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia: A Transdiagnostic Biomarker of Emotion Dysregulation and Psychopathology. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 3, 43–47 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Kondo, M. C., Jacoby, S. F. & South, E. C. Does spending time outdoors reduce stress? A review of real-time stress response to outdoor environments. Health Place 51, 136–150 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Hunter, M. R., Gillespie, B. W. & Chen, S. Y.-P. Urban Nature Experiences Reduce Stress in the Context of Daily Life Based on Salivary Biomarkers. Front. Psychol. 10, 722 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Ellett, L., Freeman, D. & Garety, P. A. The psychological effect of an urban environment on individuals with persecutory delusions: The Camberwell walk study. Schizophr. Res. 99, 77–84 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Roe, J. & Aspinall, P. The restorative benefits of walking in urban and rural settings in adults with good and poor mental health. Health Geogr. Volunt. 17, 103–113 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  65. Krieger, N., Williams, D. R. & Moss, N. E. Measuring social class in U.S. public health research: Concepts, methodologies, and guidelines. Annu. Rev. Public Health 18, 341–378 (1997).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Conger, R. D. & Donnellan, M. B. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 175–199 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Guryan, J., Hurst, E. & Kearney, M. Parental Education and Parental Time with Children. J. Econ. Perspect. 22, 23–46 (2008).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

What is the stress reactivity model?

Stress reactivity is the capacity or tendency to respond to a stressor. It is a disposition that underlies individual differences in responses to stressors and is assumed to be a vulnerability factor for the development of diseases.

What is the result of extended periods of sympathetic arousal on an individual?

The Neurobiology of Stress Sustained activation of the sympathetic system leads to chronic high heart rate and elevated blood pressure, placing the cardiovascular system at risk. Chronic inhibition of vegetative systems such as the GI tract may put those organs at risk of injury.

What is the term that describes when stress experienced in one domain of life results in stress in the other domain for the same individual?

In spillover, stress experienced in one. domain of life results in stress in the other domain for the same individual; in. crossover, stress experienced in the workplace by the individual leads to stress. being experienced by the individual's spouse at home. We broaden Bolger et.