When the presence of others negatively affects performance it is known as?

The concept of social facilitation was first proposed by American Psychologist Norman Triplett in 1898. He was studying the performance of bike racers and noticed that cyclists who trained alongside other cyclists performed better than when they simply tried to beat their personal best time.3

In 1924, Psychologist Floyd H. Allport was the first to formally use the term “Social Facilitation,” which he defined to be “an increase in response merely from the sight or sound of others making the same movement.”4 Today, the definition includes an increase in response from others who may not be performing the same task, alongside Allport’s original definition.

Triplett’s work inspired many others to study social facilitation. A 1937 study on ants revealed that worker ants will dig three times more sand for every additional ant that is present and also digging sand.5 A study done in 1967 found that armadillos tend to eat significantly more food when there are other armadillos present.6

The studies mentioned above are all examples of co-action effects. American Psychologist John Dashiell is generally attributed with discovering the type of social facilitation called “audience effects,” since a study of his from 1935 revealed that the presence of an audience increased the performance of multiplication in adult participants, measured by the number of multiplications completed. However many more studies surrounding audience effects exist today.7

Triplett’s cycling study is widely attributed as being the first social psychology laboratory experiment.1 Afterwards, during the 1930s, many psychologists fled to the United States from Nazi Germany and played a big role in the early development of social psychology. They primarily studied persuasion and propaganda, to help the US military, but later moved on to a wider variety of social issues, including racial prejudice.12 In other words, Triplett’s research on the concept of social facilitation marked the beginning of the entire social psychology field which sees many relevant applications to our everyday lives.

From both Allport’s work in 1924 and Dashiell’s in 1935, the factors which affect performance in the presence of others were condensed into the following list by American Social Psychologist Bernard Guerin in 1993: competition (rivalry), modeling, encouragement or social reinforcement, arousal, monitorability, imitation, group membership, distraction, and evaluation. All of these factors affect our day-to-day lives, and understanding them can help us use them to reach our goals, optimally motivate ourselves, and much more.

Take, for example, audiences at a sports game. This is a prime example of social facilitation at work in the real world. According to Polish-American Psychologist Robert B. Zajonc and American Psychologist Stephen M. Sales, an audience induces arousal in the athletes, causing them to perform their dominant (i.e., most natural) response. Simple, well-practiced actions will see an increase in performance; complex, unfamiliar actions will be impaired.13  Thus, athletes that practice a lot and become familiar with different plays and skills will see an improvement in front of an audience.

Athletes are not alone in this, though. Social facilitation can be further translated into the performances by musicians, comedians, and even our own tasks can also improve in front of an audience when well-versed in our actions.

Captivated by his findings in the cyclist study mentioned above, Triplett attempted to duplicate this phenomenon by observing the behavior of children doing a fishing reel task. He found that when competing with other children, half of the participants would work more quickly, one quarter worked slower, and the remaining quarter worked at the same pace as compared to performing the same task alone. In other words, 50% of children saw an increase in performance. 50% did not.

This is not the only experiment that seemed to contradict Triplett’s initial proposal. In 1933, a study published by the American Journal of Psychology found that when participants were asked to memorize a list of nonsense syllables, their performance (measured by the number of repetitions it took to fully memorize the list) was better when they were alone, rather than in the presence of someone else.14

Zanjoc and Sales propose a way to make sense of these seemingly contradictory results. They suggested that the presence of spectators enhances an individual’s dominant response. But for the majority of participants in the 1933 study, memorizing lists of nonsense syllables was not a well-practiced behavior. So, their performance would be inhibited — not aided — by the presence of spectators. Had the task felt familiar to the participants, their performance would have improved.

From this finding, the term “social inhibition” was coined. It refers to the phenomenon that occurs when one’s performance in an unfamiliar activity decreases in the presence of others watching and is generally used in contrast with social facilitation.

Alongside Zajonc and Sales’ proposal, though, there are two other proposed explanations for the inconsistencies we see between social facilitation experiments. The first was proposed by researcher Robert S. Baron in 1986, which he called the Distraction-Conflict Theory. According to Baran, the conflict between giving attention to the person present, versus focusing on the task at hand, explains the inconsistent findings.15

The second proposed explanation comes from researcher Nickolas B. Cottrell and colleagues. They proposed that instead of the mere presence of others enhancing the dominant response, a spectating audience is necessary. That is to say, it is the feeling of being evaluated—rather than being watched—that affects one’s performance. The anxiety from knowing whether those watching us are evaluating our performance is what triggers the dominant response during the performance of a task.16

Reference Guide: Social Loafing

Although we tend to contrast social facilitation and social inhibition, our behavior changes in other ways when we go from being alone to being around others. In this piece, we take a look at social loafing: the phenomenon wherein individuals exert less effort when they are working on a collective goal. Take a deeper look to understand its relevance to society shed some light on the different things that can affect your performance.

The Stages of Change: How to Motivate, Facilitate, and Reinforce Desired Behaviors

Want to learn how to motivate yourself without solely relying on the presence of others? This short read will help you understand how to apply behavioral insights to break bad habits and form new ones using motivation, facilitation, and reinforcement strategies.

Why Do We Change Our Behavior When We’re Being Watched?

Have you ever noticed that you act differently when you’re alone versus when you’re in public? This piece spotlights the observer expectancy effect, which refers to how the perceived expectations of an observer can influence the people being observed. Give this piece a quick read to learn about why the observer expectancy bias is important, and how to avoid it.

How does the presence of others affect performance?

The Yerkes-Dodson law, when applied to social facilitation, states that “the mere presence of other people will enhance the performance in speed and accuracy of well-practised tasks, but will degrade in the performance of less familiar tasks.” When compared to a person's performance alone, the individual will perform ...

What is it called when the presence of others causes improved performance?

Social facilitation, also known as the audience effect, refers to the idea that an individual's performance of a task improves in the presence of others (or while being watched).

Is the performance in presence of others?

What is social facilitation? Social facilitation is a psychological phenomenon where the performance of an individual improves in the presence of others. A competitor, an audience, or even a mere spectator can cause such an effect.

What are the three effects of others presence?

Individual behavior and decision making can be influenced by the presence of others. There are both positive and negative implications of group influence on individual behavior. While there are many ways a group can influence behavior, there are three key phenomena: groupthink, groupshift, and deindividuation.